Latest News

How to Avoid Ruining your Dog’s Recall

There is nothing more frustrating than trying to chase a dog that plays "keep away" when you are trying to get him/her to come. In the show ring, the dogs sit quietly, totally intent on watching their owner and then fly back toward them when they hear the magic word "come". 

A highly reliable recall - meaning your dog will come to you when called despite distractions - has many benefits. It can be a life saver such as when your dog escapes and encounters traffic, you come across a snake during a bushwalk or even to avoid a potential confrontation at the off leash dog park. 

THE DISASTER...

It’s a beautiful day and a couple and their two dogs are walking across a wide open field at a popular off-lead park. Gradually one of the dogs strays off, apparently following an irresistible scent near the trees. When the people finally notice that their dog has gone missing, the man shouts the dog’s name. The dog immediately lifts his head, briefly looks around to locate the source of the sound and, as the man shouts a second time, promptly runs back to his people.



When I witnessed this scene the other day I was impressed. Despite being distracted and quite far away, the dog responded instantly to his person’s voice and quickly returned. In my book this was a “perfect” recall (coming when called). However, as I watched the dog return to his family, things did not unfold as I expected. 

Instead of praising and rewarding the dog for his stellar performance, the man immediately commanded the dog into a sit, grabbed his muzzle and gave him a stern talking to. 

Now, I have seen worse being done to a dog for so-called “disobedience” but I was very disappointed by the man’s reaction. Apparently he was completely oblivious to the missed opportunity and the potential damage he had just done. What was going on here?

THE ANALYSIS

The answer may be found in the nature of the human mind and in particular its capacity to ponder the perceptions, thoughts, feelings and intentions of others. Known as Theory of Mind, this mechanism allows humans to attribute mental states to other people as well as themselves. It plays an important role in social interactions as it allows us to understand, predict and feel for others, but also to manipulate, judge and deceive others. 

Unfortunately we also routinely believe that we know what non-human animals think or feel and this is when Theory of Mind becomes a liability. Here is what Daniel Povinelli, a prominent chimpanzee researcher, has to say about the ‘distorted’ human view of animal minds:

“Faced with the overwhelming similarity in the spontaneous, everyday behaviour of humans and chimpanzees, how can someone like me – someone who has dedicated his life to studying these remarkable animals – entertain the possibility that their minds are, in profound respects, radically different from our own? How can I challenge the received wisdom of Darwin – confirmed by my own initial impressions – that the mental life of chimpanzees is best compared to that of a human child?

Actually it’s easy: I have learned to have more respect for them than that. I have come to see that we distort their true nature by conceiving of their minds as smaller, duller, less talkative versions of our own. Casting aside these insidious assumptions has been difficult, but it has allowed me to see more clearly that the human mind is not the gold standard against which other minds must be judged. For me it has also illuminated the possibility of creating a science that is less contaminated by our deeply anthropocentric intuitions about the nature of minds.”


If even science is “contaminated” with anthropocentrism, as Povinelli puts it, then maybe it’s not so surprising that most people treat their dogs like human children who need to learn “obedience”. 

Without necessarily subscribing to the idea that the minds of chimpanzees and other animals are radically different, I think Povinelli points to an important problem not just in animal research but in the wider public, namely the perception of animal minds as lesser versions of the human mind

Humans have a long history of viewing animals as less refined and less intelligent life forms while considering themselves as the “crown of evolution”. Fortunately, we are seeing a shift away from this sense of supremacy and towards a better understanding that non-human animals are not less developed but simply different compared to humans. Each species has their own distinct features, optimised for the environment they naturally live in, and their behaviour and abilities only make sense when viewed in that context.

Understanding the voice detector area in the dog brain - Photo Credit: Eniko Kubinyi

THE HUMAN PERSPECTIVE

So, going back to the scene at the park, what can be said about the man’s behaviour towards his dog and what are the potential ramifications?

By scolding the dog after calling him back the man demonstrated:

  • moral judgement and an inappropriate application of Theory of Mind – he seemingly made assumptions about the dog’s thoughts and intentions, i.e. the man obviously believed the dog made a conscious decision to follow a scent instead of following his people despite knowing it was the wrong thing to do;
  • anthropomorphism – he treated the dog like one would a disobedient child and attempted to lecture the dog about his apparent wrongdoing;
  • a lack of understanding of the principles of animal learning, specifically the behaviour-consequence contingency which states that consequences affect the probability of the preceding behaviour to occur in the future, i.e. negative consequences make the preceding behaviour less likely to happen in the future and positive consequences make it more likely.

THE DOG'S PERSPECTIVE

Trying not to fall into the same trap of assuming to know what the dog was thinking, here is my take on what the experience might have been like for the dog.

1. The dog picks up a scent and follows it. This is perfectly normal dog behaviour without which his wild ancestors would not have survived. 

To assume that the dog would ponder the consequences of his actions other than hoping to find something of interest is without foundation. 

Once on the trail the dog continues undisturbed since neither do his people pay attention nor does the dog seem to be aware of the separation from his family.


2. The dog hears his person’s voice and immediately responds to it, looking slightly confused as if woken from a slumber. He then runs to his family as soon as he spots them in the open field.

Maybe whatever had drawn his attention previously was not that appealing after all. Or maybe he was worried because he didn’t know where his family was. Maybe he had previously been trained successfully to come when called or maybe a bit of everything contributed to his quick response – who knows?


Although the dog seems at first happy to run back to his family, his body language changes as he gets closer. 

3. On approach he lowers his tail and assumes a slightly cowering posture suggesting he is uncomfortable or even afraid. He stands still as the man holds him by the muzzle and talks to him. Afterwards the dog continues for a short while with tail low before returning to normal.

At any stage the dog seems to directly respond to the current events in his environment: the scent that causes him to change path, the voice that makes him pay attention, the sight of his family which makes him happily run to them, his person’s threatening body language which makes him cower. 

There is no reason to believe, and no research to support it, that the dog understood that following the scent was somehow ‘wrong’ and that he will refrain from doing so in the future because his human got angry. 

In fact, according to everything we know about animal learning today, the dog will most likely associate approaching his person with a negative experience and avoid that person in the future in a similar context (assuming the dog experienced the muzzle grab and ‘lecture’ he received as punishing which – judging by the dog’s body language – seemed to be the case). 
I’m quite sure this is not what the man intended.

THE SOLUTION


The only way to prevent such disasters is through knowledge and awareness. By actively keeping one’s own mind in check whenever the temptation arises to make assumptions about the thoughts and intentions of another species, solutions to real or perceived problems could become a lot easier and – most importantly – a lot more humane.

So, here is what a correct application of animal learning principles looks like when teaching your dog the recall:


#1. Call your dog (happy voice)

If your dog comes to you, then reward your dog like there is no tomorrow !


#2. If your dog doesn’t come

* try again when your dog is less distracted
* try again when your dog is closer to you
* try again when your dog is hungry 



If you have built a strong history of reinforcement for coming when called, your dog is much more likely to come running back to you no matter what the circumstances. If you want to look like a pro at the dog park, reward your dog’s stellar performance instead of punishing them for non-compliance.

Adapted from Sylvie Martin's October 2014 post "How to avoid totally ruining your dog’s recall (coming when called)"

Sylvie Martin completed her training with the National Dog Trainers Federation of Australia in 2007 and graduated from the Academy for Dog Trainers in 2014. She's a member of the APDT (Association of Pet Dog Trainers Australia) & PPGA (The Pet Professional Guild Australia). 

She is the owner of Crosspaws and is committed to delivering quality dog training and behaviour modification, focussing on positive reinforcement and using science-based methods.
« PREV
NEXT »

No comments

Post a Comment